Call for papersCall for Papers International conference “Prosody at the crossroads of disciplinary pathways” Thursday 21 and Friday 22 May, 2026, Université Grenoble Alpes, France (Saint-Martin-d’Hères campus, IMAG building) Over the past decades, the enthusiasm generated by prosodic studies has spread well outside the boundaries of the traditional subfields of linguistics (phonetics, phonology, syntax, semantics and pragmatics) and reached the related disciplines of psycholinguistics, neurolinguistics, clinical and forensic practice as well as language processing (cf. Di Cristo, 2004). Whereas prosody has traditionally been used as an umbrella term covering the interconnected phenomena of stress, rhythm and intonation (whose phonetic expressions mainly involve changes in fundamental frequency, intensity and duration and their perceptual correlates [Lehiste, 1970; Arvaniti, 2020]), we also aim to include voice quality in the program alongside the established categories of stress and intonation, to encourage discussion on the nature of prosodic features. Intonation is a supralexical phenomenon consisting in variations in fundamental frequency and their perceptual correlates observed at sentence or constituent level (Ladd, 1996; Cruttenden, 1997). It can fulfil all three essential functions of speech: linguistic, paralinguistic and extralinguistic (Abercrombie, 1967). The rhythm of languages or varieties of languages relates to the hierarchical organisation of variably salient speech units in the temporal development of the production of the speech chain (Dellwo, 2003). The nature of these units varies depending on languages or language varieties. Along with Laver (1968, 1980, 1994), Sharpe (1970) and Mackenzie-Beck (2005), we consider that voice quality – which also conveys linguistic, paralinguistic and extralinguistic information – does not only result from speakers’ biologically-derived differences in vocal apparatus, but also from articulatory (orsupralaryngeal) as well as phonatory (or laryngeal) settings. Articulatory setting consists in the overall positioning of the articulatory organs. Wilson (2006) defines it as the “underlying or default posture of the articulators (i.e., the tongue, jaw, and lips)”, whereas Honikman (1964) considers that it consists in “the gross oral posture and mechanics [requisite as a framework for the integrating of the isolated sounds into that whole] which constitutes the pronunciation of a language”. This also applies to every idiolect. Phonatory settings consist in the way the vocal folds are made to vibrate. Stuart-Smith (2004) describes them as “glottal configurations” or “stricture types”; that is, the potential combination of specific types of tensions that can be brought to bear on the vocal folds. Laver (1994) established a typology of the various phonatory settings that can be achieved through these means. Prosody is traditionally defined as a set of elements whose function is superimposed upon that of the intrinsic features of segments (Lehiste, 1970). However, the growing literature establishing that children’s acquisition of prosodic structure far predates that of discrete units like phonemes and words (cf. e.g., Davis et al., 2000; Polzehl et al., 2024) suggests that it is preferable to conceive of it as an underlying matrix into which the segments are embedded. The main aim of this conference is to promote and enhance collaborations between researchers from different subdomains in order to review and discuss the applications of prosodic research to such fields as language acquisition, foreign language teaching, forensic and clinical phonetics, voice recognition, speech synthesis and sociolinguistics. Despite the existence of a growing literature on the subject, voice quality remains by far the most under-investigated of the elements listed above, especially as it has not conventionally been recognised as a component of prosody. Hence our desire to lay particular emphasis on its structural makeup and description as well as on existing assessment protocols (cf. e.g., San Segundo and Mompean, 2017, San Segundo, 2021). Participants are invited to submit proposals for both oral and poster presentations on the following issues and other related topics:
Even though the talks will be given in English, they may bear on any language(s). Submissions must include a title and a one-page anonymous abstract (excluding references), and be submitted via SciencesConf (at https://ugaprosody2026.sciencesconf.org/submission/submit) by February 16, 2026. (You will need to create a sciencesconf account if you don’t already have one.) Oral Presentations will consist of 30-minute presentations followed by 10-minute question and discussion sessions. Posters should be prepared in Portrait format with a maximum size of A0. Submission guidelines are provided at https://doc.sciencesconf.org/en/deposer/deposer.html.
REFERENCES ABERCROMBIE, D. (1967). Elements of General Phonetics. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. ARVANITI, A. (2020). “The Phonetics of Prosody.” In Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics (online).Retrieved 14 Jan. 2025, from https://oxfordre.com/lin-guistics/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.001.0001/acrefore-9780199384655-e-411. CRUTTENDEN, A. (1997 [1986]). Intonation (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DAVIS, B. L., MACNEILAGE, P. F., MATYEAR, C. L., & POWELL, J. K. (2000). “Prosodic correlates of stress in babbling: An acoustical study”. Child Development, 71, 1258–1270. DELLWO, V. (2003). “Rhythm & Speech Rate: A variation coefficient for delta C”. Proceedings of the 38th linguistic Colloquium, Budapest. DI CRISTO, A. (2004). “La prosodie au carrefour de la phonétique, de la phonologie et de l’articulation formes-fonctions”. Travaux interdisciplinaires du Laboratoire parole et langage d'Aix-en-Provence, vol. 23, pp. 67-211. HONIKMAN, B. (1964). “Articulatory Settings”. In D. ABERCROMBIE, D. B. FRY, P. A. D. MACCARTHY, N. C. SCOTT & J. L. M. TRIM (eds.), In Honour of Daniel Jones: Papers Contributed on the Occasion of His Eightieth Birthday 12 September 1961, 73-84. LAVER, J. (1968). “Voice quality and indexical information”, British Journal of Disorders of Communication 3, 43-54. LAVER, J. (1980). The Phonetic description of voice quality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. LAVER, J. (1994). Principles of phonetics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. LEHISTE, I. (1970) Suprasegmentals. Cambridge, Massachussets: MIT Press. MACKENZIE-BECK, J. M. (2005). “Perceptual analysis of voice quality: the place of Vocal Profile Analysis”. In W. J. HARDCASTLE and J. M. MACKENZIE-BECK (eds.), A Figure of Speech: a Festchrift for John Laver. London: Laurence Erlbaum, 285-322. POLZEHL, T., HERZIG, T., WICKE, F., WERMKE, K., KHAMSEHASHARI, R., DAHLEM, M., MÖLLER, S. (2024). “Towards Classifying Mother Tongue from Infant Cries – Findings Substantiating Prenatal Learning Theory”. Proc. Interspeech 2024, 4199-4203. SAN SEGUNDO & MOMPEAN (2017). “A simplified vocal profile analysis protocol for the assessment of voice quality and speaker similarity.” Journal of Voice, vol. 31, no. 5, pp. 644-e11-644-e27. SAN SEGUNDO, E. (2021). “International survey on voice quality: Forensic practitioners versus voice therapists”. Estudios de Fonética Experimental, Vol. XXX, pp. 9-34. SHARPE, M. C. (1970). “Voice quality: A suggested framework for description and some observations”. In S. A. WURM and D. C. LAYCOCK (eds.), Pacific Linguistic Studies in Honour of Arthur Capell. Pacific Linguistics Series C, 13, 115-132. STUART-SMITH, J. (2004). Phonetics and philology – Sound Change in Italic. Oxford: Oxford University Press. WILSON, I. L. (2006). “Articulatory settings of French and English monolingual and bilingual speakers”. Unpublished PhD Thesis. University of British Columbia. |
Online user: 2 | Privacy | Accessibility |
![]() ![]() |